Elsevier

Psychoneuroendocrinology

Volume 89, March 2018, Pages 120-130
Psychoneuroendocrinology

Effects of testosterone dose on spatial memory among castrated adult male rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.12.025Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Testosterone replacement restored spatial working memory in castrated male rats.

  • Testosterone replacement had no effect on reference memory in castrated male rats.

  • Testosterone replacement improved long-term memory in castrated male rats.

  • High and low physiological doses of testosterone had positive effects on memory.

  • A supra-physiological dose of testosterone had some positive effects on memory.

Abstract

Previous research on the activational effects of testosterone on spatial memory has produced mixed results, possibly because such effects are dose-dependent. We tested a wide range of testosterone doses using two spatial memory tasks: a working-reference memory version of the radial-arm maze (RAM) and an object location memory task (OLMT). Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats were castrated or sham-castrated and given daily injections of drug vehicle (Oil Sham and Oil GDX) or one of four doses of testosterone propionate (0.125, 0.250, 0.500, and 1.000 mg T) beginning seven days before the first day of behavioral tests and continuing throughout testing. For the RAM, four arms of the maze were consistently baited on each day of testing. Testosterone had a significant effect on working memory on the RAM, with the Oil Sham, 0.125 mg T, and 0.500 mg T groups performing better than the Oil GDX group. In contrast, there was no significant effect of testosterone on spatial reference memory on the RAM. For the OLMT, we tested long-term memory using a 2 h inter-trial interval between first exposure to two identical objects and re-exposure after one object had been moved. Only the 0.125 and 0.500 mg T groups showed a significant increase in exploration of the moved object during the testing trials, indicating better memory than all other groups. Testosterone replacement restored spatial memory among castrated male rats on both behavioral tasks, but there was a complex dose-response relationship; therefore, the therapeutic value of testosterone is likely sensitive to dose.

Introduction

Spatial memory, the ability to recall the locations and relative spatial relationships of stimuli in one’s environment, shows a robust sex difference in humans (Voyer et al., 1995), with men performing better than women on a variety of spatial tasks (Kaufman, 2007; Woolley et al., 2010). Similarly, male rats have better spatial memory than females based on performance in the Morris water maze (MWM) (Markowska, 1999), radial-arm maze (RAM) (Gibbs and Johnson, 2008), and object location memory task (OLMT) (Cost et al., 2012). Considerable evidence indicates that male sex steroids (androgens) play an important role in producing sex differences in spatial memory. Experiments with humans and rats have shown that testosterone has organizational effects upon the brain early in development that enhance spatial learning and memory (Bull et al., 2010; Isgor and Sengelaub, 1998). Whether androgens have activational effects that enhance spatial memory in adult men remains controversial (Holland et al., 2011; Ulubaev et al., 2009), but some studies have shown a positive correlation between circulating testosterone levels and performance on a variety of spatial memory tasks both in younger men, under 30 years old (Christiansen and Knussmann, 1987; Silverman et al., 1999; Thilers et al., 2006), and in older men, over 50 years old (Barrett-Connor et al., 1999; Moffat et al., 2002; Yaffe et al., 2002). Among patients with prostate cancer, androgen deprivation therapy caused significant impairments in spatial ability (Cherrier et al., 2009). However, testosterone replacement given to healthy elderly men has produced mixed effects on spatial cognition (Emmelot-Vonk et al., 2008), so the therapeutic benefits of testosterone replacement remain unclear.

Studies with rats have shown that castration in adulthood impairs spatial memory (Gibbs and Johnson, 2008; Kritzer et al., 2001; Spritzer et al., 2008), and exogenous testosterone replacement restores the spatial memory of castrated adult male rats to normal (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003; Spritzer et al., 2011). Additionally, testosterone seems to differentially influence the working and reference components of spatial memory. Within this context, working memory is defined as a form of short-term memory that involves storage of information from a particular task for as long as it is useful to complete the task, and reference memory is defined as the long-term storage of memories that are used from one task to the next (Cowan, 2008; Olton and Papas, 1979). Although most studies show no effect of castration on reference memory on the RAM (Gibbs and Johnson, 2008; Spritzer et al., 2008), some experiments involving the MWM indicate that testosterone replacement can improve spatial reference memory in adult male rats (Khalil et al., 2005; Spritzer et al., 2011). In contrast, testosterone replacement has consistently been shown to improve spatial working memory on various versions of the RAM (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003; Gibbs and Johnson, 2008; Spritzer et al., 2008). However, the reference vs. working memory dichotomy does not fully capture the variety of spatial memory tasks that have been employed with rodents. For example, two experiments have shown that testosterone injections improve performance on the OLMT with a 2 h delay interval (Jacome et al., 2016; McConnell et al., 2012), suggesting that testosterone improves reference (i.e., long-term) memory when the retention period is relatively short. Thus, although the most consistent activational benefits of testosterone have been demonstrated using working-memory tasks, testosterone may also improve some forms of long-term memory.

It has been speculated that the relationship between testosterone and spatial memory is curvilinear, with both unusually low and high levels of testosterone causing memory impairments. In support of this hypothesis, long-term use of anabolic-androgenic steroids has been shown to impair memory in humans (Kaufman et al., 2015). Similarly, intra-hippocampal injections of supra-physiological doses of testosterone impair spatial memory in male rats (Emamian et al., 2010). Some studies with men have supported the optimum-testosterone hypothesis (Muller et al., 2005; Nowak et al., 2014), but few studies have tested a broad range of testosterone doses on spatial memory using rodents. In our previous work, we found that a broad range of testosterone doses improved performance on the MWM relative to castrated control rats, and certain doses (0.250 and 1.00 mg/rat) impaired reversal learning (Spritzer et al., 2011). Another study using the MWM demonstrated a curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial reference memory, with a high physiological dose (0.750 mg/rat) of testosterone optimal for performance (Jia et al., 2013).

In the current study, we tested the effects of both testosterone elimination and replacement, using four doses of testosterone, ranging from physiological to supra-physiological, on the performance of adult male rats on two different tasks: a working-reference memory version of the eight-arm RAM and an OLMT. For our RAM experiment, we predicted that testosterone replacement would restore working memory in castrated male rats while having minimal impact on reference memory (Spritzer et al., 2011). The OLMT has been used in relatively few studies testing the relationship between testosterone and memory (Jacome et al., 2016; McConnell et al., 2012). Two advantages of this task are that it avoids the stressors associated with other tasks and that the inter-trial interval can be easily varied to test different memory durations. We used a 2 h inter-trial interval on the OLMT for comparison with the longer retention period (24 h) that we used for RAM testing. Thus, these experiments replicate and expand upon past research designed to assess the cognitive benefits of androgen replacement.

Section snippets

Subjects

Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (approximately 60 days old) were acquired from Charles River Laboratories (St. Constant, Quebec, Canada). Animals were individually housed in opaque, polypropylene bins (21 × 42 × 21 cm) with metal lids and Tek-Fresh Bedding (Harlan Laboratories, Indianapolis, IN, USA). Rats had free access to water (glass water bottles) and soy protein-free food (Harlan Teklad Diet 2020X), except during periods of food restriction for RAM testing. The housing and testing rooms

Experiment 1: testosterone had dose-dependent effects on RAM performance

All of the rats from the Oil GDX group had serum testosterone concentrations below the detection limit for the assay (0.05 ng/ml) except one sample that was just above the detection limit (0.057 ng/ml). Due to low variance, this group was not included in statistical comparisons. Serum testosterone concentrations differed significantly among the other groups (Fig. 2A; F4,67 = 83.47, p < 0.0005). Post-hoc comparisons showed that each group had significantly different serum testosterone

Discussion

Although the cognitive effects of testosterone on men remain ambiguous (Puts et al., 2010; Young et al., 2010), substantial research has shown that androgen replacement therapy provides cognitive benefits for some clinical populations (Cherrier et al., 2009; Nead et al., 2017). Our results demonstrated that testosterone had some dose-dependent effects on spatial memory in male rats. On the 8-arm RAM, castration impaired spatial working memory, and some doses of testosterone restored working

Conflicts of interest

None.

Funding

This project was funded by Middlebury College, the Vermont Genetics Network (INBRE grant number 2P20RR016462) from the INBRE Program of the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), and the National Institute of Aging (NIH AREA grant number 1R15AG042155). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of funding agencies. The funding agencies had no role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data;

Contributors

B.A Wagner, V.C. Braddick and M.D. Spritzer contributed to the design of this study and to the writing of the manuscript. B.A. Wagner, V.C. Braddick, C.B. Batson, B.H. Cullen, and L.E. Miller all made substantial contributions to data collection. All co-authors contributed to data analysis and interpretation, provided input on revisions of this manuscript, and approved the final version of the submitted article.

Acknowledgements

We thank Melissa Childs, Christina Chyr, Emily Goins, Eliza Jaeger, Charlotte Michaelcheck, Cyrus Jalai, Rita Pfeiffer, Amanda Reis, and Summer Spillane for their assistance with data collection. We thank Vicki Major and the animal care staff for their assistance, and we thank Mark Stefani and Tony Desautels for help with the design and construction of the open field and RAM. Jan Thompson helped us develop the OLMT protocol.

References (68)

  • R.B. Gibbs

    Testosterone and estradiol produce different effects on cognitive performance in male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2005)
  • W.R. Hawley et al.

    Testosterone modulates spatial recognition memory in male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • J. Holland et al.

    Testosterone levels and cognition in elderly men: a review

    Maturitas

    (2011)
  • C. Isgor et al.

    Prenatal gonadal steroids affect adult spatial behavior, CA1 and CA3 cell morphology in rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (1998)
  • J. Jia et al.

    Amelioratory effects of testosterone treatment on cognitive performance deficits induced by soluble Aβ1-42 oligomers injected into the hippocampus

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • B.A. Jones et al.

    Spatial memory performance in androgen insensitive male rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2005)
  • M.J. Kaufman et al.

    Brain and cognition abnormalities in long-term anabolic-androgenic steroid users

    Drug Alcohol Depend.

    (2015)
  • S.B. Kaufman

    Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity?

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • M.F. Kritzer et al.

    Gonadectomy impairs T-maze acquisition in adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2001)
  • M.N. Locklear et al.

    Assessment of the effects of sex and sex hormones on spatial cognition in adult rats using the Barnes maze

    Horm. Behav.

    (2014)
  • M.N. Locklear et al.

    Local N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor antagonism in the prefrontal cortex attenuates spatial cognitive deficits induced by gonadectomy in adult male rats

    Neuroscience

    (2015)
  • V. Luine et al.

    Effects of estradiol on radial arm maze performance of young and aged rats

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1994)
  • N.J. MacLusky et al.

    Androgen modulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • R.H. Matousek et al.

    A randomized controlled trial of add-back estrogen or placebo on cognition in men with prostate cancer receiving an antiandrogen and a gonadotropin-releasing hormone analog

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2010)
  • S.E.A. McConnell et al.

    The role of testicular hormones and luteinizing hormone in spatial memory in adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2012)
  • N.T. Nowak et al.

    Contributions of sex, testosterone, and androgen receptor CAG repeat number to virtual Morris water maze performance

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • D.S. Olton et al.

    Spatial memory and hippocampal function

    Neuropsychologia

    (1979)
  • M.G. Packard et al.

    Inactivation of the hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1996)
  • V.K. Patchev et al.

    Neurotropic action of androgens: principles, mechanisms and novel targets

    Exp. Gerontol., Proceedings of the Seventh International Symposium on the Neurobiology and Neuroendocrinology of Aging

    (2004)
  • D.A. Puts et al.

    Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women

    Horm. Behav.

    (2010)
  • A. Rizk et al.

    Behavioral performance of tfm mice supports the beneficial role of androgen receptors in spatial learning and memory

    Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • I. Silverman et al.

    Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (1999)
  • M.D. Spritzer et al.

    Effects of testosterone on spatial learning and memory in adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2011)
  • M.D. Spritzer et al.

    Testosterone influences spatial strategy preferences among adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • Cited by (31)

    • Sex-dependent differences in animal cognition

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text